• Skip to primary navigation
  • Skip to main content
  • Skip to primary sidebar
The Analysis Factor

The Analysis Factor

Statistical Consulting, Resources, and Statistics Workshops for Researchers

  • Home
  • Our Programs
    • Membership
    • Online Workshops
    • Free Webinars
    • Consulting Services
  • About
    • Our Team
    • Our Core Values
    • Our Privacy Policy
    • Employment
    • Collaborate with Us
  • Statistical Resources
  • Contact
  • Blog
  • Login

How to Understand a Risk Ratio of Less than 1

by Audrey Schnell 2 Comments

When a model has a binary outcome, one common effect size is a risk ratio. As a reminder, a risk ratio is simply a ratio of two probabilities. (The risk ratio is also called relative risk.)

Risk ratios are a bit trickier to interpret when they are less than one. 

A predictor variable with a risk ratio of less than one is often labeled a “protective factor” (at least in Epidemiology). This can be confusing because in our typical understanding of those terms, it makes no sense that a risk be protective.

So how can a RISK be protective?

Well, by indicating lower risk. 

For example, let’s say you’re running a model where the outcome is Conviction of a Felony (yes/no) and among your predictors are Previous Criminal Activity (yes/no) and Graduation from High School (yes/no).

We would expect that a Yes on Previous Criminal Activity is related to an increase in the risk of committing a felony. Likewise, we would expect that a Yes on Graduation from High School is related to a decrease in the risk of committing a felony. 

In other words, Previous Criminal Activity would be a risk factor and Graduation from High School would be a protective factor. Yet the effect of both factors would be measured with a risk ratio.

The risk ratio is always defined as the ratio of the comparison category’s probability to the reference category’s probability.

A risk ratio greater than one means the comparison category indicates increased risk.

A risk ratio less than one means the comparison category is protective (i.e., decreased risk).

Say we have the following data for a group of defendants:

 

Felony Conviction

Graduation

Yes

No

Total

No

300

100

400

Yes

225

175

400

Total

525

275

800

 

From this table, we can calculate the probability that either a graduate or a dropout is convicted of a felony.

P(Felony conviction|Dropout) = 300/400 = .75
P(Felony conviction|Graduate) = 225/400 = .5625

And from those, we can calculate the risk ratio for graduates compared to dropouts.

RR:  Graduates/Dropouts =.5625/.75 = .75

As you can see, the probability of a felony conviction is lower for graduates (.5625) than it is for dropouts (.75). Likewise, the risk ratio of felony convictions for graduates compared to dropouts is less than one (.75).

So one interpretation is that graduation is protective — it is associated with a lower risk of conviction.

How much lower? By a factor of .75, or 25% lower risk.

Now if we reversed this comparison, we could say that dropping out of high school increases risk and therefore is a risk factor. We would do this by swapping the comparison and recalculating the risk ratio:

RR  Dropouts/Graduates = .75/.56 = 1.33

Here we conclude that dropouts are 33% more likely than graduates to be convicted of a felony.

Some references will advise re-coding the data so that the relative risk is always greater than 1. However, it is important to take into consideration the message you want to deliver. In the example above, it may make sense to drive home the message that graduates are 25% less likely to be convicted.

If, after your initial analysis, you find the risk ratios counterintuitive, you can recode the reference group so that the interpretation makes sense.

 

Binary, Ordinal, and Multinomial Logistic Regression for Categorical Outcomes
Get beyond the frustration of learning odds ratios, logit link functions, and proportional odds assumptions on your own. See the incredible usefulness of logistic regression and categorical data analysis in this one-hour training.

Tagged With: binary outcome, predictor variable, probability, protective factor, relative risk, risk ratio

Related Posts

  • The Difference Between Relative Risk and Odds Ratios
  • When Linear Models Don’t Fit Your Data, Now What?
  • Logistic Regression Analysis: Understanding Odds and Probability
  • Member Training: Explaining Logistic Regression Results to Non-Researchers

Reader Interactions

Comments

  1. Donaldino Green says

    October 22, 2019 at 12:02 pm

    Great example given. Really appreciate this article. Helped me a lot!

    Reply
  2. Edouard says

    February 28, 2019 at 7:42 pm

    I actually think of it in a different manner. First in my mind initially there are only factors which will then prove to be a risk or a protective depending on the association. In theory a factor could be a risk in one context but protective in another context. What I think of is the natural state of the subject. The process involved is what happens if I were to pour a factor into a subject that would alter their natural state. Hence people were not born to be smokers. Then the natural unexposed group is a non-smoker. What happens if I were to load a person with smoking (exposed group) and then examine the association relative to the natural state. In this case, we tend to see a risk or odds ratio greater than unity. In that case, I say that the factor poses a risk hence becomes a risk factor. On the other hand, people are not born to be educated. It is a conscious effort that societies have learnt (since the Industrial Revolution) to be desirable. Then the natural state is a non-educated person (unexposed) but the more education poured into that person (exposed) tends to yield a risk or odds ratio less than one. Then the conclusion is that adding education to the person or to populations tend to yield a protective result hence labelled a protective factor. It is very rare that across studies factors are risk in one context and protective in others. Across most studies, the relationship is generally invariant. One possible long time held such cross over was smoking — being a risk factor for most diseases but apparently a protective factor for dementia or Alzheimer’s disease. However this has been debunked in the WHO “Tobacco Knowledge Summary on Tobacco and Dementia” which argued that a meta analysis of studies funded by the Tobacco Industry yielded smoking as a protective factor whereas a meta analysis of non-Tobacco Industry funded studies yielded smoking as a risk factor.

    Reply

Leave a Reply Cancel reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

Please note that, due to the large number of comments submitted, any questions on problems related to a personal study/project will not be answered. We suggest joining Statistically Speaking, where you have access to a private forum and more resources 24/7.

Primary Sidebar

This Month’s Statistically Speaking Live Training

  • Member Training: Analyzing Pre-Post Data

Upcoming Free Webinars

Poisson and Negative Binomial Regression Models for Count Data

Upcoming Workshops

  • Analyzing Count Data: Poisson, Negative Binomial, and Other Essential Models (Jul 2022)
  • Introduction to Generalized Linear Mixed Models (Jul 2022)

Copyright © 2008–2022 The Analysis Factor, LLC. All rights reserved.
877-272-8096   Contact Us

The Analysis Factor uses cookies to ensure that we give you the best experience of our website. If you continue we assume that you consent to receive cookies on all websites from The Analysis Factor.
Continue Privacy Policy
Privacy & Cookies Policy

Privacy Overview

This website uses cookies to improve your experience while you navigate through the website. Out of these, the cookies that are categorized as necessary are stored on your browser as they are essential for the working of basic functionalities of the website. We also use third-party cookies that help us analyze and understand how you use this website. These cookies will be stored in your browser only with your consent. You also have the option to opt-out of these cookies. But opting out of some of these cookies may affect your browsing experience.
Necessary
Always Enabled
Necessary cookies are absolutely essential for the website to function properly. This category only includes cookies that ensures basic functionalities and security features of the website. These cookies do not store any personal information.
Non-necessary
Any cookies that may not be particularly necessary for the website to function and is used specifically to collect user personal data via analytics, ads, other embedded contents are termed as non-necessary cookies. It is mandatory to procure user consent prior to running these cookies on your website.
SAVE & ACCEPT